By Jim Geraghty
Friday, May 15, 2020
On the menu today: We could all dunk on people who were
wrong in their early assessments of this pandemic, but the more pressing
question is who is not learning from getting things wrong during this outbreak;
one mainstream publication notices that the conventional wisdom on Florida was
far from the truth; why the media’s reflexive partisan sympathies are leading
Americans to not understand the virus and what policies work best to mitigate
it; and a funny and brutally honest assessment of what’s coming to higher
education.
Errors Are a Part of
Life, but We Shouldn’t Cling to Them
According to the Johns Hopkins University Coronavirus
Research Center, as of this writing, the U.S. death toll from the virus is
85,906. Worldometers, which tends to run ahead of JHU count, puts the toll at
86,912.
You noticed that the arguments of “It’s just the flu!” or
“This is comparable to the flu!” or “The flu kills more people each year!”
stopped sometime in the past few weeks, didn’t you? Even the skeptics can
count. (As our Robert VerBruggen noted, even that “the flu kills 80,000
Americans per year” statistic is a debatable statistical estimate.)
You may recall that at the end of March, President Trump
said, “so if we can hold that down, as we’re saying, to 100,000, it’s a
horrible number, maybe even less, but to 100,000, so we have between 100
[thousand] and 200,000, we altogether have done a very good job.” Many
commentators, particularly on the Left, thought Trump had not merely moved the
goalposts from his past statements that the virus would go away, or that it was
not as dangerous as the flu, but was setting an absurdly low bar for a “good
job.” At the end of March, the United States had barely 4,000 deaths. Surely,
Trump had to be setting expectations at an unrealistically high number, so he
could come back later and claim victory, right?
No, apparently not!
You may recall that by April 8, when deaths were at about
17,000, the IHME model — which week by week proved to be just too flawed to be
useful — revised its projection to indicate the virus would kill 60,000 people
in the United States over the next four months. That revision was 33,000 less
than a week earlier, prompting many to argue that either the model was simply
too imprecise to give any meaningful conclusions, or that the virus was less
dangerous than initially thought.
It’s five weeks later, and we’re at 85,906.
I think highly of Dr. Anthony Fauci and think just about
everything he says during this outbreak is worth careful consideration. But
there’s no getting around the fact that some of his early assessments were
really off base, too. On January 21, the day the first U.S. case was
discovered, Fauci said during an interview, “obviously, you need to take it
seriously and do the kind of things the (Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention) and the Department of Homeland Security is doing. But this is not a
major threat to the people of the United States and this is not something that
the citizens of the United States right now should be worried about.” In
another interview on January 26, he repeated, “The American people should not
be worried or frightened by this. It’s a very, very low risk to the United
States, but it’s something we, as public health officials, need to take very
seriously.”
They call it a novel coronavirus because it’s new,
not because everyone is supposed to finish writing a novel during quarantine.
Because it is new, all of us are trying to understand it, and our previous
experiences may or may not be applicable to this virus and this outbreak. I
suspect that the way countries reacted to SARS-CoV-2 was heavily shaped by
their experiences with the original SARS, H1N1, Middle East Respiratory
Syndrome, Ebola virus, and Zika. Hong Kong, South Korea, Japan, Taiwan,
Singapore — all of those countries’ populations have endured much more severe
outbreaks of contagious diseases in recent years. All of those countries’
populations have been conditioned to take any reports, or even rumors, about
contagious diseases seriously. Other than H1N1, those previous outbreaks barely
affected the United States and its citizens. Our experience told us that
viruses in far-off lands almost never become a serious problem in this country.
The Hoover Institution’s Richard Epstein got an enormous
amount of grief for writing an essay posted on March 16 about policies for the
outbreak, initially estimating that the toll of the coronavirus would be only
500 people, and then revising it upward to 5,000, and then later to 50,000.
Epstein called it, “the single largest unforced intellectual error in my entire
academic career, when I included numerical estimates about the possible impact
of the coronavirus in terms of life and death. Those estimates were obviously
ridiculously too low.”
Epstein’s numbers were wildly off-base, but his concluding
point in that essay wasn’t cuckoo for Cocoa Puffs:
The first point is to target
interventions where needed, toward high-risk populations, including older
people and other people with health conditions that render them more
susceptible to disease. But the current organized panic in the United States
does not seem justified on the best reading of the data. In dealing with this
point, it is critical to note that the rapid decline in the incidence of new
cases and death in China suggests that cases in Italy will not continue to rise
exponentially over the next several weeks. Moreover, it is unlikely that the
healthcare system in the United States will be compromised in the same fashion
as the Italian healthcare system in the wake of its quick viral spread. The
amount of voluntary and forced separation in the United States has gotten very
extensive very quickly, which should influence rates of infection sooner rather
than later.
All of us, from the president and Fauci to the kids down
the street are trying to grapple with the unknown. Just about all of us are
going to get something wrong at some point. Here we are, May 15, and we’re
still not entirely sure whether children are largely immune to this virus, or
whether some portion will develop “multisystem inflammatory syndrome” some
months or weeks later. (The current leading theory is that this is some sort of
delayed reaction by a child’s immune system after fighting off the virus.)
Thankfully, this syndrome appears to be unlikely to kill children.
We think we’re less likely to catch the virus outside —
it may be much, much less likely. Vitamin D might be a factor — or maybe it’s a
more general sense that the vitamin is just good for your immune system in
general. We’re not sure how long the antibodies against this virus will stay in
human bodies. We’re pretty sure masks help, but we’re not sure how much, or how
effectively people will wear them. A prominent virologist thinks he caught the
virus through his eyes on a crowded flight because he was wearing a mask
and gloves the entire time. It appears humans can spread the virus to dogs, but
dogs cannot spread the virus to people.
What we know can change. Perhaps our appetite for rubbing
someone’s nose in their getting something wrong has created an enormous
disincentive for anyone ever admitting they’re wrong — and an inadvertent
incentive for stubbornly clinging to an assessment, even in the face of
mounting counter-evidence.
No comments:
Post a Comment